National Council of Teachers of Mathematics 2012 Research Presession

Please note: The NCTM conference program is subject to change.

96- Children’s Informal Conceptions of Integers

Wednesday, April 25, 2012: 1:00 PM-2:30 PM
Franklin Hall 10 (Philadelphia Marriott Downtown)
Perspective and Educational Importance

In most integer-related research, researchers (a) document students’ errors, misconceptions, or ways of approaching certain types of problems (Chiu, 2001; Gallardo, 2002; Peled (1991); Thomaidis & Tzanakis, 2007; Vlassis, 2008) or (b) propose instructional models or contexts for teaching integers and operations on integers (Janvier, 1983; Liebeck, 1990; Linchevski & Williams, 1999; Streefland, 1996; Thompson & Dreyfus, 1988). The majority of integer research falls into the latter category, with description of a variety of instructional models, tools, and contexts for teaching integers (e.g., scores and forfeits, number lines, microworlds). Much of the former category of integer-related research documents the significant challenges middle and high school students have in either understanding or correctly responding to integer-related tasks (see, for example, Gallardo, 2002; NAEP, 1996; Thomaidis & Tzanakis, 2007; Vlassis, 2008).

Integers and integer operations are building blocks for success with algebra. They mark a transition from arithmetic to algebra because of their abstract nature (Hefendehl-Hebeker, 1991; Linchevski & Williams, 1999); and to navigate algebraic equations, students must perform algebraic procedures using additive inverses, which first come into play with the introduction of integers. Although research shows that students struggle to make sense of integers, to date, few researchers have focused on students’ ways of reasoning about integers (exceptions include Behrend & Mohs (2006) and Wilcox (2008)). We focus on Grades 2 and 4 students’ ways of reasoning about integers because, as a field, we have little knowledge about the ways in which young children approach this mathematical topic. Further, we believe that by understanding students’ informal, preinstructional ways of reasoning, teachers, educational researchers, and curriculum designers can build on these ways so that students can develop more sophisticated reasonings about integers. 

In our research design and analyses, we take a children’s mathematical thinking perspective (Carpenter et. al, 1999; Empson & Levi, 2011). Thus, in starting with how students make sense of mathematics, we try to uncover their ways of reasoning about integers.

Methods

Participants

We are reporting on a subset of a large, cross-sectional, multi-grade-level study. In this work session, we report findings from interviews with Grades 2 and 4 students only. Participants included 40 students in each of Grades 2 and 4 from seven schools in San Diego County, selected to represent a broad cross-section of students according to ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and scores on California Standardized Tests.  None of these 2nd- and 4th-grade students had received formal, school instruction on integers. Our goal was to document these children’s informal and intuitive integer understandings prior to school-based instruction.

Interview Tasks

We developed tasks specific to integer reasoning for problem-solving interviews on the bases of an historical analysis of integer development and a review of the relevant literature. We posed open number sentences (i.e., problems of the form -3 + c = 6 and c + 6 = 4), varying the location of the unknown. We also posed comparison tasks (e.g., Which is larger, -7 or -8?) and problems in context. The hour-long clinical interviews were conducted during the school day.

Analysis

We began analyzing interviews with a process of open coding (Strauss & Corbin, 1998), focusing on students’ solution strategies and underlying ways of reasoning about number and operations. We used the constant-comparative method to identify emergent, distinguishing themes and features of students’ reasoning about integers. We have found that for young children who had negative numbers in their conceptual domains (that is, students who knew of negative numbers), their responses fell primarily into one of three categories of reasoning about integers—leveraging the ideas of order, magnitude, or formalisms. Though individual children did not typically use a single way of reasoning across all problems, many children were consistent in their problem-solving approaches and invoked a primary way of reasoning. Thus, we find useful considering how a particular view of number, consistently applied across problems, both supported and constrained students’ integer understanding.

We also consider the relative difficulty of problems for the students by examining patterns of reasoning (and correctness) both within and across students. Understanding which tasks are more challenging (or easier) than others and hypothesizing about what makes some tasks more challenging than others can inform sequencing of tasks for teachers and curriculum designers.

Session Organization

In this work session we seek to engage the audience in a subset of the findings from a cross-sectional study designed to document students’ conceptions of integers. Presenters will share findings about Grades 2 and 4 students’ conceptions and relative problem difficulty and will engage the audience in extended discussion about the findings and elicit their ideas about implications. The session will serve as an occasion to share with the community young children’s ways of reasoning about integers, an area that has received scant attention in the literature.

5 minutes        Session and speaker introduction

50 minutes      Presentation and audience discussion of Grades 2 and 4 students’ ways of reasoning

During this time we will show several carefully chosen video clips. After each clip, the audience will share what they notice and react to findings. As appropriate, we will facilitate discussions about students’ underlying integer conceptions and relative problem difficulty.

30 minutes      Audience discussion about implications of findings

During this time we will orchestrate conversations about implications for teaching and for further research.

5 minutes        Wrap up and audience debrief of session

Five Central Questions

  1. What do our findings about Grades 2 and 4 students’ strategies suggest about the students’ conceptions of integers?
  2. What was the relative level of difficulty of tasks for Grades 2 and 4 students, and what task characteristics affected the level of difficulty?
  3. In what ways do findings resonate with audience members? What findings are surprising?
  4. What are possible implications of the findings about young children’s informal conceptions of integers for teachers and teacher educators?
  5. What are possible implications of the findings about young children’s informal conceptions of integers for researchers?

References

Behrend, J., & Mohs, C. (2006). From simple questions to powerful connections: A two-year  conversation about negative numbers. Teaching Children Mathematics, 12, 260–264.   

Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E., Franke, M. L., Levi, L., & Empson, S. B. (1999). Children’s

mathematics: Cognitively guided instruction. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Chiu, M. M. (2001). Using metaphors to understand and solve arithmetic problems: Novices and

experts working with negative numbers. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 3, 93–

124.

Empson, S. B., & Levi, L. (2011). Extending children’s mathematics: Fractions and decimals.

Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Gallardo, A. (2002). The extension of the natural-number domain to the integers in the transition

from arithmetic to algebra. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 49, 171–192.

Hefendehl-Hebeker, L. (1991). Negative numbers: Obstacles in their evolution from intuitive to

intellectual constructs. For the Learning of Mathematics, 11, 26–32.

Janvier, C. (1983). The understanding of directed number.  In J. C. Bergeron & N. Herscovics

(Eds.), Proceedings of the Fifth Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the

International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 295– 301).  Montreal: Universite de Montreal, Faculte de Sciences de l’Education.

Liebeck, P. (1990). Scores and forfeits—An intuitive model for integer arithmetic. Educational

Studies in Mathematics, 21, 221–239.

Linchevski, L., & Williams, J. (1999). Using intuition from everyday life in ‘filling’ the gap in

children’s extension of their number concept to include the negative numbers.

Educational Studies in Mathematics, 39, 131–147.

National Assessment of Educational Progress. (1996). Test items released on NAEP web site, http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/naepdata/

Peled, I. (1991). Levels of knowledge about signed numbers:  Effects of age and ability. Proceedings of the Fifteenth Psychology of Mathematics Education Conference, 3, 145–152.

Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for

developing grounded theory (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Streefland, L. (1996). Negative numbers: Reflections of a learning researcher. Journal of

Mathematical Behavior, 15, 57–77.

Thomaidis, Y., & Tzanakis, C. (2007). The notion of historical “parallelism” revisited: Historical

evolution and students’ conception of the order relation on the number line. Educational

Studies in Mathematics, 66, 165–183.

Thompson, P. W., & Dreyfus, T. (1988). Integers as transformations. Journal for Research in

Mathematics Education, 19, 115–133.

Vlassis, J. (2008). The role of mathematical symbols in the development of number

 conceptualization: The case of the minus sign. Philosophical Psychology 21, 555–570.

Wilcox, V. B. (2008). Questioning zero and negative numbers.  Teaching Children Mathematics, 15, 202–206.

Speaker:
Lisa Clement Lamb
Co-speakers:
Jessica Pierson, PhD , Randolph A. Philipp , Ian Whitacre , Bonnie Schappelle and Melinda Lewis


Description of Presentation:

This interactive session will draw on video of children to share research findings from 80 hour-long interviews with grades 2 and 4 students about their ways of reasoning about integers. Audience members will react to the findings and will help identify implications for teaching and research.

Session Type: Work Session

See more of: Work Session
<< Previous Presentation | Next Presentation >>